Hearing helps the brain stay in rhythm better than touch
10-17-2025

Hearing helps the brain stay in rhythm better than touch

Close your eyes and tap to your favorite song – you’ll probably stay right on beat. Try the same thing with vibrations through your fingertips, and the rhythm will start to drift. That’s because the brain keeps steadier time with sound than with touch.

A Belgian research team has now mapped out how and why, comparing sound with patterns of fingertip vibrations in healthy adults. Their tests revealed clear differences in how the brain builds a beat from each type of input.

Hearing holds the beat

Lead researcher Cédric Lenoir at UCLouvain studied how the brain handles rhythm delivered by speakers versus fingertip buzzers.

The team recorded brain signals and tapping while people followed the same patterns from the ear or from touch.

When listening, the brain formed slow, beat-like fluctuations, and people tapped more steadily, according to a new preprint comparing acoustic and tactile rhythm under matched conditions.

The same setup showed that touch responses mostly tracked each vibration burst, with less stable timing.

“The ability to move in time with a beat is essential for human social interactions through music,” said Lenoir. “Future research will help clarify whether long-term music practice can strengthen the brain’s ability to process rhythm through other senses.”

Tracking beats in the brain

The team used electroencephalography to track brain activity while volunteers tapped a finger in time with each rhythm. Identical patterns were presented either as tones from a speaker or as fingertip vibrations.

For sound, slow brain activity tied to the perceived beat emerged below 15 hertz, while touch responses spread into higher frequencies up to about 25 hertz. People also tapped less precisely when following the vibrations.

This difference reflects how the brain tracks rhythm through a process called neural entrainment – its ability to align internal activity with an external beat.

Prior research has shown that the strength of this synchronization varies across people and predicts how accurately they move in time.

In hearing, networks linking auditory areas to motor timing hubs seem tuned to low frequencies that match musical beats, helping the brain compress fast notes into simpler, movement-ready patterns.

Brain favors hearing for rhythm

Touch can support timing, but it may not recruit the same slow, integrative circuit that hearing does. In the new work, vibrotactile signals were tracked more literally, one event at a time.

Other groups have shown that people can synchronize to vibrotactile rhythms and show related brain responses.

That skill – called sensorimotor synchronization – coordinating movement precisely to a rhythmic cue, appears less stable than with sound in many contexts.

Slower beats, stronger sync

The critical difference seems tied to frequency. Hearing produces strong low-frequency activity that mirrors the perceived beat, while touch emphasizes the faster event rate.

Independent work finds that rhythmic brain responses often show selective boosts at the beat rate.

One report suggests this enhancement depends on tempo, reinforcing the idea that the brain privileges certain slow ranges that match comfortable human movement.

Everyday music pulls on these same circuits. Studies in natural listening show that the brain tracks structure in songs and that people share similar timing patterns across listeners.

Slow beats guide movement

Slow brain rhythms connect to loops that help the body start and stop actions on time. That makes them useful for coordinating a finger tap or a dance step.

This low-frequency window also aligns with natural gait cycles and with timing used in speech. When hearing locks into this range, movement tends to fall in line.

Healing through rhythm

Because hearing accesses this slow, integrative code, music can serve as a potent timing cue in clinical settings. Therapies that use beats to guide walking or arm movement build on that strength.

Reviews of music and brain circuitry note that rhythmic cues can shape motor networks over weeks of practice. One such review highlights how auditory rhythms engage motor planning areas crucial for rehabilitation.

Sound leads, touch adapts

The new results suggest that timing tools should match the sense they rely on. For precise coordination, sound still has the edge – most people keep steadier time with what they hear than what they feel.

Yet touch isn’t out of the game. Devices that convert sound into richer vibration patterns could improve timing by emphasizing slow components that the brain interprets as a beat.

Future research will test how far that can go. Long training might strengthen the brain’s ability to build a beat from touch alone, or reveal how hearing loss shifts people’s reliance on tactile cues in daily life.

Scientists can also explore ways to boost slow rhythmic activity during touch – using gentle movement or visual downbeats – to see whether the brain can learn to treat nonauditory signals more like music.

What’s next for rhythm science

This study examined healthy adults using controlled lab rhythms. Results may differ when people hear messy, real-world music or when they move their whole body.

Individual differences also matter. Some people may naturally rely more on touch or vision when they keep time, and training can tilt that balance.

Hearing holds a privileged route to the beat, producing slow, behavior ready brain activity that touch does not consistently provide. That edge helps explain why tapping to music feels so easy and why therapy often starts with sound.

The study is published in the journal JNeurosci.

—–

Like what you read? Subscribe to our newsletter for engaging articles, exclusive content, and the latest updates. 

Check us out on EarthSnap, a free app brought to you by Eric Ralls and Earth.com.

—–

News coming your way
The biggest news about our planet delivered to you each day
Subscribe